Development
of a comprehensive financial literacy scale
Desarrollo de una escala integral de
educación financiera
Nuria Patricia, Rojas-Vargas[1], Julio César, Vega-Mendez[2]
Financial
literacy and money management practices are fundamental aspects of economic
growth. After developing a literature review, we analyze the most important
aspects of the terminology and propose an integral definition of the financial
literacy concept. The final goal of this paper is to develop a comprehensive
scale to measure financial literacy as defined by the authors according to the
evolution of the concept, taking into account related scales to money
management found in the literature, and applicable to a wide range of people.
The authors develop a structural equation modeling to relate unobserved
constructs to observed variables and validate the scale with a divergent and
convergent analysis.
Key words: Financial literacy, scale development,
literature review
Códigos JEL: G53, G51, D14, I22,
C38.
The financial literacy landscape has become more
sophisticated over the past few years with the introduction of many new
financial products and services (Cull & Whitton, 2011; Deepak et al. 2015),
a growing number of workers approaching retirement (Kamakia,
et al. 2017), undesired financial and economic consequences in the economy (Refera et al, 2016; Cichowicz
& Nowak, 2017), high levels of poverty (Nanda & Samantha, 2018) and
rising levels of household debt (Cull & Whitton, 2011).
However, several financial problems can be found in
the micro-level; people do not have the basic financial knowledge, and when
they do, they do not implement it. Financial literacy is remarkably important
as it can improve the standard of living (Lindsey-Taliefero
et al. 2011), individuals must plan for financial security that extends 20 or
30 years further (Faulkner, 2015). According to Fortuna (2007), Americans have
poor financial habits; a large percentage of the population lacks basic
financial knowledge and skills to ensure long-term stability for themselves and
their families. In Mexico this is not far from true, according to the ENIF
(2015), only 41.2 percent of the Mexican population has a financial retirement
product to save for the future and only just 36 percent keeps a record of their
expenses.
Previous research has looked into
different aspects of managing personal finances and money. Stango and Zinman (2009) stipulated that people choose to consume,
borrow, or save based on their preferences, their expectations, and the cost
and benefits of borrowing and saving. We can find different scales that measure
the competences of people toward their financial behavior (Yamauchi & Templer, 1982; Spinella, Yang
& Lester, 2007). However, the objective of this paper is to introduce a
comprehensive approach to this discussion of financial literacy: the
application of the knowledge on every day’s lives. We aim to assess if the
origins of bad financial practices are in the lack of knowledge of how to
manage money or at the stage of application of the knowledge. Therefore, we
seek to develop a scale that integrates the level of personal financial
knowledge and its application to manage their money.
Financial literacy reflects the development of an
economy. Therefore, it is important to realize the impact that good management
of the financial resources can make in people's lives overall. Remembering the
2008 crisis, it was personal mortgages defaults which originated the biggest
financial crisis since the Great Depression (Mian
and Sufi, 2009).
Building on this, the present study attempts to
develop a comprehensive financial literacy scale with the objective to explore
the financial knowledge applicable to a more general population than previous
financial literacy scales or related matters used in early studies (Atikson & Messy, 2011) and other related personal
finance scales (Spinella, Yang & Lester, 2007).
The implementation of this scale is made in the Mexican population from 24 to
38 years old with recurrent income.
Nevertheless, the present measurement instrument can be applied in other
regions and ages.
The remainder of the paper is structured as follows.
First, we develop a literature review of the financial literacy concept and
discuss the findings classified separated by five major areas. Then we searched
for scales related to personal finances to base our scale development. Building
on this, we present the elaboration of a comprehensive scale. The next section
describes the data and methods, we detail our findings on the development of
the scale. The final section provides the conclusion and future research
directions.
To provide an overview of the relevant and current
research literature that defines the concept and subject areas analyzed under
this terminology, an integrative literature review was conducted by the authors
following the methodology of Torraco (2005; 2016).
The concept Financial Literacy was determined as the relevant research topic,
determined as the jointed keywords.
For this search, we used databases of academic
literature such as EBSCO, ProQuest, Scopus, Web of Science and Google Scholar.
We focused the research on works published in the English language and it does
not cover books or non-academic research papers. This methodology yielded 26
research articles in total. The references of the selected articles were then
examined, via snowballing technique, to find relevant literature related to the
target concept. The final set of articles reviewed was 51. Information was mostly retrieved from the
introduction, discussion and conclusion sections of the works reviewed. We
looked mainly for the relevance of the concept, how authors defined it, areas
mentioned in the field, factors taken into account
when conducting studies, and measurement methods and issues present in
financial literacy studies.
Importance of the Financial Literacy Concept
The financial crisis of 2007 shifted attention of the
world towards its importance to ordinary and sophisticated investors (Abdullah
& Chong, 2014, Kebede & Kuar, 2015). Other
aspects that nowadays are attracting attention to the subject matter are the
growing number of workers approaching retirement (Kamakia
et al. 2017) and the recent shift of retirement responsibility from governments
to individuals (Refera & Kaur, 2016).
It has been proven that lack of financial knowledge
leads to poor choice and decision making, which can result in undesired
financial and economic consequences to the individual, the financial system and
the general economy (Refera, et al. 2016). Financial
security can only be achieved when the population is considered financially
literate (Taylor & Wagland, 2011). Therefore,
financial literacy leads to correct financial decision making and independence
(Charitha, 2018). It has been found that households
with higher levels of financial literacy are better at reacting to a shock like
the financial crisis (Bucher-Koenen, T., & Ziegelmeyer, 2011). When people are financially literate
their current decisions provide support and prepare them for an uncertain
future.
Evolution of the Financial Literacy Concept
There is confusion regarding the definition of the
term financial literacy (Faulkner, 2015). According to Remund
(2010), a clearer definition would improve future research, as it would provide
the basis for the studies in the areas to cover, the measurement aspects and so
forth. Differences in the definitions had led to different measurements which
in turn have caused mixed results (Kamakia et al.
2017). Selim and Aydemir (2014) proposed that studies
should primarily describe financial literacy to set the ground before
proceeding to another stage of research.
Even though the definition of the concept has been
advancing, the main idea has prevailed. In its origins, in the 1990s, seminal
authors in the topic talked about the ability of an individual to make
judgements and effective decisions regarding the use of money (Noctor, Stoney & Strading,
1992). More than 10 years later, in the twenty first century, authors were
talking about a process by which people acquires knowledge and develop the
skills required to make those effective choices in this topic (OCDE, 2005).
After 2010, the level of definitions found in the literature increased
exponentially, authors started mixing more and more subject areas such as
awareness, different types of knowledge (e.g. financial products, concepts, etc), skills, attitudes, and behavior (Cull & Whitton,
2011; Huston, 2010). The latest years, authors highlighted the importance of
the application of this basic knowledge to make informed decisions (Amagir, A., Groot, W., Van Den Brink, H., & Wilschut, A., 2018).
Based on this integrative research we propose the
following definition:
“Financial literacy encompasses the basic knowledge of
concepts and products related to their usage of money throughout a person life,
the skills to apply this knowledge and look for direction when requiring
specialized guidance, their attitude and behavior towards the different areas
that they should take into account when planning for their future”.
Areas of the Financial Literacy Concept
The concept encompass several areas about finance,
according to the research a broad array of elements integrate the financial
literacy concept: planning or budgeting (Taylor & Wagland,
2011; Vieira, 2012; Totenhagen et al. 2015; Amagir, et al. 2018), savings (Chen & Volpe, 1998;
Lusardi, 2006; Fortuna, 2007; Lindsey-Taliefero, et
al. 2011; Taylor & Wagland, 2011; Vieira, 2012; Totenhagen et al. 2015; Refera,
et al. 2016; Amagir, et al. 2018), investing (Chen
& Volpe, 1998; Lusardi, 2006; Fortuna, 2007; Lindsey-Taliefero,
et al. 2011; Taylor & Wagland, 2011; Cull &
Whitton, 2011; Deepak et al. 2015; Totenhagen et al.
2015; Amagir, et al. 2018), spending (Taylor & Wagland, 2011; Vieira, 2012; Amagir,
et al. 2018), borrowing (Chen & Volpe, 1998; Fortuna, 2007; Lindsey-Taliefero, et al. 2011; Refera,
et al. 2016), insurance (Chen & Volpe, 1998; Fortuna, 2007; Refera, et al. 2016; Amagir, et
al. 2018), and planning for retirement or superannuation (Lusardi, 2006; Taylor
& Wagland, 2011; Cull & Whitton, 2011; Collins
& O’Rourke, 2012; Vieira, 2012; Deepak et al. 2015). Finally, few point out to areas as money management (Lindsey-Taliefero, et al. 2011; Refera,
et al. 2016) which may encompass all the previous areas.
Some authors argue that all basic education should
include a varied of topics pointing to planning or budgeting, saving, spending,
investing and credit (Totenhagen et al. 2015; Amagir et al. 2018) being saving and investing the ones
that need the greatest improvement (Lindsey-Taliefero,
2011).
Factors considered in Financial Literacy
Studies
Ratna
et al. (2018) provides a long list of factors that influence the financial
literacy. Some can be summarized as demographic factors (e.g. gender,
education, age, among others.) additionally, there are some others that can be
comprised as previous experience on the subject (money attitude, perception and
opinion, and so on). Deepak et al. (2015) highlights the importance of
identifying predictors of financial literacy and establishes that the most
important are financial education, cognitive ability, maturity and family
background.
Deepak et al. (2015) have conclude that the major
factors of financial literacy are financial knowledge, financial behavior and
financial attitude.
Measurement methods and issues in Financial
Literacy Studies
Researchers commonly employ questionnaires to test the
financial literacy of individuals (Fortuna, 2007; Lindsey-Taliefero
et al. 2011; Charitha, 2018). The questionnaires
often encompass financial literacy areas (e.g. saving, investing, borrowing,
etc.) questioning mainly about knowledge, as well as, demographic factors. The
processing of the data extracted from the surveys has primarily been done by
cross-sectional or longitudinal methods with regression analysis (Lindsey-Taliefero et al. 2011).
However, Financial literacy has been measured in
several different ways (Selim & Aydemir, 2014), a
unified financial literacy conceptualization is urgent to unify its measurement
(Kimiyaghalam & Safari, 2015) and compare among studies
to provide generalizable findings.
An additional research was done to discover scales in
subjects related to financial literacy to identify the ways of how questions in
the subject are done. For this search, we used the database of academic
literature EBSCOhost. We selected academic articles that fit the specific
keywords: “personal finance scale”, “personal attitudes towards money”,
“attitudes toward managing money” and “personal money management scale”. In
this search, these keywords have been considered for the complete research
articles, i.e. title, abstract and text. These keywords fulfill the task to
keep the focus on relevant scales concerning the measurement of attitudes
toward managing money.
Out of the papers identified based on these keywords,
in a second step, we look through the complete articles searching for the
scales mentioned or based their research on. This methodology yielded five
scales in total. We searched for the articles that developed the scales founded
to assess their objectives and content. A brief description of the scales, the
authors, and item examples are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Scales measuring
attitudes towards money
Scale |
Authors |
Description |
Item
Example |
Money Attitudes Scale |
Yamauchi & Templer, 1982 |
The scale provides a reliable
assessment of five factors of money attitudes. |
I do financial planning for the
future. |
Compulsive Buying Scale |
Faber & O'Guinn,
1992 |
Unidimensional scale to identify
compulsive buyers. |
If I have any money left at the
end of the pay period, I just have to spend
it. |
Material Values Scale |
Richins & Dawson, 1992 |
Materialism scale with three
components. |
The things I own say a lot about
how well I’m doing in life. |
Executive Personal Finance Scale |
Spinella, Yang & Lester,
2007 |
Self-rating of executive aspects
of personal money management. |
When I see something I want,
I have a hard time not buying it. |
Perceptions of payment mode scale |
Khan, Belk & Craig-Lees, 2015 |
Captures consumers perceptions in
19-item four dimensions. |
If I had a 100 note in my
wallet... I would feel confident. |
Source.
Elaborated by the authors
The Money Attitudes Scale (Yamauchi & Templer, 1982) provides a reliable assessment of five
factors of money attitudes: Power-prestige, Retention-time, Distrust, Quality,
and Anxiety. The response format of the scale is a 7-point Likert scale,
constituted by 29 items. This scale can be utilized to identify irrational and
problematic attitudes and behaviors with money. Further research has applied
this scale to measure compulsive buying in young Mexican adults (Roberts &
Sepulveda, 1999). Other authors have tested the consistency of undergraduates
and community residents (Yang & Lester, 2002; Spinella,
Lester & Yang, 2005).
The Compulsive Buying Scale (Faber & O'Guinn, 1992) is a unidimensional scale composed by seven
items to identify compulsive buyers by represented behaviors, motivations, and
feelings associated with buying significantly. It is stated that compulsive
buying becomes very difficult to stop and ultimately results in harmful
economic, psychological and societal consequences. This scale has been applied
to analyze the severity concept of compulsive buying in a sample of 44 subjects
considered compulsive buyers. Results have come to the
conclusion that compulsive buyers with lower incomes had greater illness
severity and were less likely to have incomes above the median (Black, Monahan,
Schlosser & Repertinger, 2001). An additional
study has compared the scale with another two compulsive buying scales in an
Italian sample, concluding that this scale has a better validity measuring
compulsive buying in survey research. (Tommasi & Busonera, 2012).
Material Values Scale (Richins
& Dawson, 1992) is a scale to measure materialism among individuals with
three components. Acquisition centrality, when people places possessions and
their acquisition at the center of their lives; acquisition as the pursuit of
happiness, when the pursuit of happiness is through acquisition rather than
through other means; and possession-defined success, when people judge their
own and others success based on the number and quality possessions accumulated.
We have found that Richins
(2002) developed a short form of the Material Values Scale (MAS), with 15 items
that improve the dimension properties. This scale has been tested in a
cross-cultural study measuring consumers among Eastern and Western Europe,
concluding that a new instrument is needed to measure equivalent materialism in
a cross-cultural context (Griffin, Babin,
Christensen, 2004). Moreover, adaptations of this scale have been performed to
be applied in children developing the scale MVS-c (Opree
SJ., et al, 2011).
Executive Personal Finance Scale (Spinella,
Yang & Lester, 2007) is a self-rating of executive aspects of personal
money management. Twenty items are grouped into 4 factors: impulse control,
organization, planning, motivational drive. The scale showed to had
correlations with compulsive buying and money attitudes. The study is based on
ample evidence that executive functions, and the prefrontal systems of the
brain that mediate them, play a role in managing personal finances. This allows
the behavior of goal-oriented, flexible, and autonomous. Authors analyze
demographic influences, one variable was education, and it had no apparent
impact on the total score, it is important to declare education as years of
general education, not financial education. Items were created to reflect
different domains of finances, organization, financial planning, and impulse
control over spending.
Additional publications about the previous scale
performed an analysis using 138 college students, concluding that the planning subscale
appeared to consist of two distinct components, investment, and saving behavior
(Lester, Spinella, 2007). Recently, a validity study
of the scale was performed in 93 undergraduate students obtaining results that
support the Executive Personal Finance Scale (Yang & Lester, 2016).
The Perceptions of payment mode scale (PPM) (Khan,
Belk, & Craig-Lees, 2015) captures the cognitive and emotional associations
with payment modes. Composed of 19 items, this scale represents four
dimensions: emotions relating to cash payment, emotions related to card-based
payment, social and personal gratification and money management. According to
the authors, the scale can aid researchers to know how cognitive and emotional
associations affect spending behavior.
Thus far, we have not found any adaptations to this scale in the
literature or applications in different contexts, the scale is relatively new
and has six citations according to ScienceDirect.
Based on these previous developments, we aim to create
a scale that integrates the key aspects of our proposed concept: financial
literacy. The scale will examine the existing scales to analyze if there items
that can be extracted to their implementation in the comprehensive financial
literacy scale. Additionally, it will integrate new items to measure financial
literacy based on theoretical approaches and advice from experts in the field.
Following the proposed definition of financial
literacy and with guidance from experts in the personal finance field, we
establish the following dimensions for the construction of the comprehensive
financial literacy scale. The following dimensions are proposed from the
literature and have been used in some money management scales.
Expenditures. In this dimension Keown et al. (2003), explain that
is important for every individual to have a financial plan. Kapoor et al.
(2009) establish the importance of detailing your living expenses and other
financial obligations in a spending plan.
Credit Card. For Keown et al. (2003) the most dangerous debt is
right in your pocket, your credit card. When people use them most of the times,
they do not think through, as they do not need to exchange cash. Also, they may
become addicted to spend with this resource. However, the authors point the
benefits of owning a credit card if used smartly; they facilitate online
purchases, they assist in tracking spending for budgeting purposes, and some of
them provide insurances in travels and personal accidents.
Investment. Investment has been a dimension when evaluating
personal finance knowledge in several studies related to money management (Chen
et al. 2002). Nissenbaum et al. (2004) proposed
investment planning as a strategy to build wealth through the understanding of
investment vehicles and financial markets. Kapoor et al. (2009) recognize that
there are many types of investment vehicles available and people should select
them according to their financial needs.
Savings.
The savings dimension has been included in related personal-finance scales
(Chen et al. 2002). Kapoor et al. (2009) signaled that previous research
indicates that people with a financial plan had significantly higher amounts in
savings than those who did not have a plan.
Retirement. Lusardi et al. (2011). Conducted a research focused
on retirement plans, they assure that people fail to plan for retirement and
conclude that people with good financial practices are more likely to plan and
to succeed in their planning, they rely on formal methods such as retirement
calculators, retirement seminars, and financial experts, instead of family,
relatives, and co-workers.
Insurance. Adequate insurance coverage is an important component
of personal financial planning Kapoor et al. (2009). Nissenbaum
et al. (2004) stated that a way to protect your family and assets fundamental
in financial planning is through insurances, they proposed life, health,
property/causality, disability, and auto insurance.
As previously mentioned, we establish that financial
literacy in general population can be measured by obtaining information about
money practices in six areas; how do people implement their knowledge on the
subject matter in their daily lives. The initial areas proposed for the
construction of this scale were expenditures, savings, insurance, credit cards,
retirement and investments. Proxy statements were used to code these variables
using a Likert scale response for each statement. A total of 69 items were
developed for revision submission with experts. After the expert’s
recommendations a total of 29 items were considered to collect information in a
pre-test exercise.
Sample for the data collection were obtained from
general population over 18 years old with no specific characteristics. Principal
sampling sources were author’s personal network. Secondary sources include
firefighters’ station, graduate schools, parks and coffee shops. For the
pretest analysis a sample of 72 participants were used, feedback from
participants included changes in the composition of statements, rearrangement
of the options in the answer section and the introduction statement to
questionnaire.
Final distribution of questionnaire included a total
sample of 172 respondents, from which 16 were deleted because either were under
18 years old or didn’t answered all sections of the questionnaire. The
principal channel of distribution was online, only the application for the
pretest sample were done in person. Because the sensitive of the information
provided the authors were prohibited from identifying the respondents by name
or generating a mailing list.
We execute a factor analysis to determine how many
factors were necessary to group the 29 items. In our first analysis, nine
factors were obtained reaching a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.76 and an explained
variance of 51.37%, factor loads are shown in Appendix 1. After this analysis,
we obligated the execution of 6 factors with the complete number of items. The
results from the second factor analysis is shown in appendix 2.
We observed items developed for a specific dimension
grouped in other dimensions, the six factors grouped items not related to a
specific domain in the literature. Our first goal was to arrange the factors
that group the items in a manner that make sense according to our six
dimensions. We executed a reliability analysis and examine items that if
deleted from the model increase the Cronbach’s alpha, also those that showed a
factor load less than 0.60 and those that were grouped in a wrong dimension.
The items that did not accomplished the required criteria were deleted (i.e.,
Q15RC, Q4RC, Q24RC, Q6RC, Q9RC, Q11RC).
As we can notice, all deleted items were reverse code.
After this process, we executed the factor analysis to determine how many
factors were necessary to group the 23 items left. The analysis resulted in
seven factors reaching a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.829 and an explained variance of
63.94%, reaching a better model, the factor loads are shown in Appendix 3.
Results show that item Q27 is grouped alone in factor
number seven. The rest of the factors, from one to six, grouped all of the items according the dimension they belong to.
Then, we executed the model with the restriction of six dimensions, the
grouping of items did not make sense again. The reliability analysis showed
that if item Q27 were deleted from the model, the Cronbach’s alpha would
increase from 0.829 to 0.833. Based on these results we decided to remove item
Q27 to reach our first goal. We run a factor analysis with the 22 items left,
reaching a 61.11% of explained variance. The factor loads are shown in Appendix
4.
Our second goal is to improve the model by eliminating
items with low load to improve the model (i.e., Q18 and Q28). Then we executed
factor and reliability analysis to obtain the loads and Cronbach’s alpha for
our improved model with 20 items that explain the 62.36% of the variance. Loads
for this final model are shown in Table 2.
Table 2
Factor Analysis. 20 items. 6 Factors
Item |
Factor Number |
|||||
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
|
Q3 |
0.800 |
0.138 |
0.087 |
0.200 |
-0.113 |
0.022 |
Q1 |
0.798 |
0.098 |
0.010 |
0.159 |
0.128 |
0.126 |
Q5 |
0.730 |
-0.180 |
-0.014 |
0.221 |
0.145 |
-0.051 |
Q17 |
0.457 |
0.357 |
0.271 |
-0.145 |
0.146 |
0.305 |
RC Q19 |
0.024 |
0.736 |
0.095 |
0.072 |
-0.115 |
-0.102 |
RC Q20 |
0.062 |
0.708 |
-0.213 |
0.168 |
0.024 |
0.120 |
RC Q21 |
-0.094 |
0.652 |
-0.385 |
0.177 |
0.162 |
0.023 |
Q16 |
0.220 |
0.582 |
0.220 |
0.043 |
0.092 |
0.408 |
Q2 |
0.391 |
0.392 |
0.012 |
0.042 |
0.323 |
-0.045 |
Q23 |
-0.032 |
-0.097 |
0.761 |
0.141 |
0.129 |
0.120 |
Q22 |
-0.081 |
0.086 |
0.753 |
0.279 |
0.113 |
-0.061 |
Q25 |
0.250 |
-0.082 |
0.727 |
0.036 |
0.100 |
0.099 |
Q7 |
0.084 |
0.019 |
0.326 |
0.716 |
0.021 |
0.150 |
Q8 |
0.231 |
0.217 |
0.087 |
0.698 |
0.097 |
0.068 |
Q10 |
0.256 |
0.181 |
0.057 |
0.627 |
0.124 |
0.079 |
Q13 |
0.110 |
0.153 |
0.117 |
-0.098 |
0.776 |
0.157 |
Q12 |
0.069 |
0.006 |
0.107 |
0.443 |
0.684 |
0.057 |
Q14 |
0.058 |
-0.235 |
0.365 |
0.315 |
0.536 |
-0.048 |
Q26 |
-0.092 |
0.187 |
-0.047 |
0.092 |
0.011 |
0.800 |
Q29 |
0.200 |
-0.153 |
0.172 |
0.173 |
0.137 |
0.730 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
When we assessed the best model available from the
information obtained, the developed model was introduced into AMOS, to run a
structural equation model analysis. Items were renamed for simplicity. The
introduced model is shown in Figure 1, relations between constructs and the
observable variables can be identified.
Source.
Elaborated by the authors
Figure
1.Structural
Model 1
To validate our model, we estimate the Goodness of Fit
Index (GFI) by running the default model in AMOS. The GFI obtained is of 0.848,
a desirable value for GFI is of 0.90 (Revuelta, J.,
& Kessel, D., 2007), meaning that our model can be improved. Other
valuation parameters that we use to determine if our model is well adjusted to
measure the constructs are the RMSEA, the obtained value was 0.071, a desirable
value is 0.05 (Steiger & Lind, 1980). We
calculate the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) to obtain a value of 0.835, a
desirable value is 0.90 or more (Bentler, P.
M.,1990), this bring us to the same conclusion, our model can be improved.
We execute a convergent analysis to determine that the
observed variables are measuring the determined constructs (Fornell
& Larker, 1981). The estimations of the structural equation model for each
relation between variable and construct are shown in Appendix 5.
As we can see the variable Q20 has a low estimate of
0.484; the construct “Investment” is only measured by Q20 and Q19, if we delete
Q20 the construct will be measured directly from Q19 and no estimation can be
done. Then, we calculate the Average Extraction (AVE) for each construct, a
desirable value is more than 0.5, results are shown in Appendix 6.
As we can see, no value is more than 0.5; the
construct “Insurance” has the lowest value with 0.371. Then we proceed to
calculate the, results are shown in Appendix 7.
The desirable value for Composite Reliability is 0.70
or more. In our model the constructs “Credit Cards” “Savings” and “Insurance”
have a lower Composite Reliability than 0.70. The value that brings our
attention is “Insurance” with 0.53. Based on this, we decide to eliminate the
construct of “Insurance” and leave 5 dimensions measured by 18 variables. The
final model is shown in Figure 2.
Source.
Elaborated by the authors
Figure
2. Final
Structural Model
To validate our new model, we estimate the Goodness of
Fit Index (GFI). The GFI obtained improved to 0.866, closer to 0.9. The value
for RMSEA also improved to 0.069, closer to 0.05. We calculate the Comparative
Fit Index (CFI) to obtain an improved value of 0.866, closer to 0.90, this
bring us to the same conclusion; our model was improved by excluding the
insurance dimension.
We execute a convergent analysis for our new model to
determine that the observed variables are measuring our constructs. The
estimations of the structural equation model for each relation between variable
and construct are shown in Table 3.
Table 3
Convergent Analysis
Observed Variable |
|
Unobserved Construct |
Estimate |
Q1 |
<--- |
E |
0.817 |
Q2 |
<--- |
E |
0.403 |
Q3 |
<--- |
E |
0.767 |
Q4 |
<--- |
E |
0.609 |
Q5 |
<--- |
E |
0.422 |
Q6 |
<--- |
CC |
0.49 |
Q7 |
<--- |
CC |
0.542 |
Q8 |
<--- |
CC |
0.731 |
Q9 |
<--- |
CC |
0.6 |
Q10 |
<--- |
I |
0.661 |
Q11 |
<--- |
I |
0.754 |
Q12 |
<--- |
I |
0.655 |
Q13 |
<--- |
S |
0.623 |
Q14 |
<--- |
S |
0.718 |
Q15 |
<--- |
S |
0.63 |
Q16 |
<--- |
R |
0.749 |
Q17 |
<--- |
R |
0.465 |
Q18 |
<--- |
R |
0.592 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
As we can see, the
variables Q2, Q5, Q6, Q7, Q9, Q10, Q12, Q13, Q15, Q17 and Q18 have a low
estimate; less than 0.7. Then we calculate the Average Extraction (AVE) for
each construct, a desirable value is more than 0.5, results are shown in Table
4.
Table 4
AVE
Unobserved Construct |
AVE |
E |
0.3934 |
CC |
0.3571 |
INV |
0.4782 |
S |
0.4335 |
R |
0.3759 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
As we can see, all
values are less than 0.5. We then calculate the Composite Reliability,
results are shown in Table 5.
Table 5
Composite Reliability
Unobserved Construct |
Composite Reliability |
E |
0.7502 |
CC |
0.6846 |
INV |
0.7324 |
S |
0.6956 |
R |
0.6353 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
The desirable value
for Composite Reliability is 0.70 or more. In our model, the constructs “Credit
Cards” “Savings” and “Retirement” have values of Composite Reliability close to
0.7; concluding that for all the model the observed variables are measuring the
unobserved construct.
We develop a
divergent analysis (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988) to
prove that the constructs are different from each other. First, we calculate
the Chi-square for the default model and for every subsequent model placing a
constraint of total correlation between two constructs. Results are shown in
Table 6.
Table 6
Chi-square
Correlation |
Chi square |
P-Value |
Default Model |
217.12 |
|
E & CC |
289.99 |
3.8501E-49 |
E & I |
304.00 |
5.0073E-65 |
E & S |
262.30 |
4.4314E-68 |
E & R |
262.20 |
5.4098E-59 |
CC & I |
301.40 |
5.6882E-59 |
CC & S |
268.70 |
1.6322E-67 |
CC & R |
270.30 |
2.1789E-60 |
I & S |
266.10 |
9.7617E-61 |
I & R |
241.40 |
8.0338E-60 |
S & R |
238.30 |
1.9474E-54 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
The results show that all hypothesis of correlation
equal to one are rejected; concluding that the constructs are different from
each other. An additional analysis is carried out according to Fornell & Larker (1981) to prove that given any pair of
constructs, one explains more variance with the items that constitute it, than
the other construct. To compute the analysis, we need the correlations of each
pair of constructs, shown in Table 7.
Table 7
Construct correlations
Construct 1 |
|
Construct 2 |
Correlations |
E |
<--> |
CC |
0.297 |
E |
<--> |
I |
0.221 |
E |
<--> |
S |
0.538 |
E |
<--> |
R |
0.369 |
CC |
<--> |
I |
-0.162 |
CC |
<--> |
S |
0.411 |
CC |
<--> |
R |
0.096 |
I |
<--> |
S |
0.446 |
I |
<--> |
R |
0.568 |
S |
<--> |
R |
0.632 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
We based the analysis in the following criteria to
validate divergence:
It can be observed in Table 8, that for any pair of
construct, the correlation of the constructs present a lower value than the
minimum AVE of each construct, except for the pair of savings and retirement,
where the square of correlation is higher than the minimum AVE of both
constructs. This can be explained analyzing the nature of the constructs, where
one person need to save money for retirement,
nevertheless, the minimum AVE has a value close to the correlation.
Table 8
Divergence validation
Construct 1 |
Construct 2 |
(Corr)^2 |
Min AVE |
E |
CC |
0.09 |
0.36 |
E |
I |
0.05 |
0.39 |
E |
S |
0.29 |
0.39 |
E |
R |
0.14 |
0.38 |
CC |
I |
0.03 |
0.36 |
CC |
S |
0.17 |
0.36 |
CC |
R |
0.01 |
0.36 |
I |
S |
0.20 |
0.43 |
I |
R |
0.32 |
0.38 |
S |
R |
0.40 |
0.38 |
Source. Elaborated by the authors
The final scale is composed by 18 items and can be
found in Appendix 1.
The final goal of this paper is to develop a
comprehensive financial literacy scale that evaluates the practices of people
in their finances. A structural equation model was proposed to specify
weightings for eighteen variables that significantly contributed to value the
five principal dimensions on financial literacy allowing to distinguish those
persons that take wrong decisions in money management. These dimensions include
practices in expenses, savings, retirement, credit cards and investments.
This research was focus on financial literacy on the
general population, distinct as past studies in personal finances where the
primary focus was a specific population with unique characteristics (i.e.
executives, students). The study’s intension is to help other researchers
assess in a reliability manner the level of good practices in personal finances
that a specific population presents, and relate this
findings to other characteristics.
The study present limitations that need to be
acknowledged. While the results are encouraging, unfortunately, no assessment
of stability was feasible in the study because of the single contact required
by the confidentiality restriction. Another factor that need to be exposed is
the resources limitation for obtaining the sample. The authors tried to collect
the most variability in the characteristics of the individuals included in the
sample, nevertheless the time limitation caused that the most part of the
sample were from author’s personal networks.
In the study the developed scale was validated by a
convergent and divergent analysis. We encourage for future research to validate
the scale by applying it into two groups of samples. First sample including
individuals that had demonstrated good personal finance practices, and second
sample including individuals that had demonstrated bad personal finance
practices. The study can utilize a proxy like credit score to evaluate
individuals. The validation expectative would be that the screened groups
resembled the results in the scale. The Personal Finance Scale developed in
this study consist in eighteen items, which brings the possibility to adequate
a new study to develop a small version of the scale.
Abdullah, M. A. and Chong, R. (2014). Financial
Literacy: An Exploratory Review of the Literature and Future Research. Journal of Emerging Economies & Islamic
Research, 2(3).
Amagir, A., Groot, W., Maassen van den Brink, H. and
Wilschut, A. (2018). A review of financial-literacy education programs for
children and adolescents. Citizenship,
Social and Economics Education, 17(1), 56-80.
Anderson, J. C. and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural
equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological bulletin, 103(3), 411.
Atkinson, A. and Messy, F. A. (2011). Assessing
financial literacy in 12 countries: an OECD/INFE international pilot exercise. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance,
10(4), 657-665.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in
structural models. Psychological Bulletin,
107(2), 238-246.
Black, D. W., Monahan, P., Schlosser, S. and
Repertinger, S. (2001). Compulsive buying severity: an analysis of compulsive
buying scale results in 44 subjects. The
Journal of nervous and mental disease, 189(2), 123-126.
Bucher-Koenen, T. and Ziegelmeyer, M. (2011). Who lost
the most? Financial literacy, cognitive abilities, and the financial crisis. ECB,1299.
Charitha, K. L. (2018). Review of Impact of Financial
literacy and Self-confidence on Customer Decision of Accepting Financial
Advisory Services.
Chen, H. and Volpe, R. P. (2002). Gender differences in
personal financial literacy among college students. Financial services review, 11(3), 289-308.
Cichowicz, E. and Nowak, A. K. (2017). Review of
research on financial literacy and education of Poles. Journal of Insurance, Financial Markets and Consumer Protection, 26
(4/2017), 3-18
Collins, J. M. and O'Rourke, C. (2012). Still holding
out promise: A review of financial literacy education and financial counseling
studies. Network Financial Institute:
Working Paper Series.
Cull, M. and Whitton, D. (2011). University students'
financial literacy levels: obstacles and aids. The Economic and Labour Relations Review, 22(1), 99-114.
Deepak, C. A., Singh, P. and Kumar, A. (2015).
Financial literacy among investors: theory and critical review of literature. International Journal of Research in
Commerce, Economics & Management, 5 (4).
ENIF (2015). ¿Cómo
estamos en Educación Financiera? CONDUSEF. Retrieved from <a href=”https://www.condusef.gob.mx/Revista/PDF-s/2016/197/enif.pdf” target="_blank"> https://www.condusef.gob.mx/Revista/PDF-s/2016/197/enif.pdf </a>
Faber, R. J. and O'guinn, T. C. (1992). A clinical
screener for compulsive buying. Journal
of consumer Research, 19(3), 459-469.
Faulkner, A. E. (2015). A systematic review of
financial literacy as a termed concept: More questions than answers. Journal of Business & Finance
Librarianship, 20(1-2), 7-26.
Fornell, C. and Larker, D. (1981). Structural equation
modeling and regression: guidelines for research practice. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50.
Furtuna, F. (2007). College students’ personal
financial literacy: Economic impact and public policy implications. Undergraduate Economic Review, 4(1), 1.
Griffin, M., Babin, B.J. and Christensen, F. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of the materialism construct:
Assessing Richins and Dawson's materialism scale in Denmark, France and
Russia. J Bus Res., 57(8), 893–900. <a href= “http://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(02)00290-4” target="_blank"> http://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(02)00290-4 </a>
Huston, S.J. (2010). Measuring financial literacy. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 44(2), 296-316.
Kamakia, M. G., Mwangi, C. I. and Mwangi, M. (2017).
Financial Literacy and Financial Wellbeing of Public Sector Employees: A
Critical Literature Review. European
Scientific Journal, ESJ, 13 (16), 233.
Kapoor, J. R., Dlabay, L. R. and Hughes, R. J. (2009). Personal finance (9th edition). Irwin.
Kebede, M. and Kuar, J. (2015). Financial Literacy and
Management of Personal Finance: A Review of Recent Literatures. Research Journal of Finance and Accounting,
6(13), 92-106.
Keown, A. J. and Hanna, S. D. (2003). Personal Finance:
Turning Money Into Wealth. Journal of
Financial Counseling and Planning (2), 93-94.
Khan, J., Belk, R. W. and Craig-Lees, M. (2015).
Measuring consumer perceptions of payment mode. Journal of Economic Psychology, 47, 34-49.
Lester, D. and Spinella, M. (2007). The executive personal finance scale: Item analyses. Psychological Reports, 101(3), 722-722. <a href=”http://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.101.3.722” target="_blank"> http://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.101.3.722 </a>
Lindsey-Taliefero, D., Kelly, L., Brent, W. and Price,
R. (2011). A Review of Howard University's Financial Literacy Curriculum. American Journal of Business Education, 4(10),
73-84.
Lusardi, A. (2006). Financial literacy and financial
education: Review and policy implications. NFI
Policy Brief, (2006-PB), 11.
Lusardi, A. and Mitchell, O. S. (2011). Financial literacy and planning:
Implications for retirement wellbeing (No. w17078). National Bureau of
Economic Research.
Mian, A. and Sufi, A. (2009). The consequences of
mortgage credit expansion: Evidence from the US mortgage default crisis. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124(4),
1449-1496.
Nanda, A. K. and Samanta, S. (2018). Mainstreaming
tribals through financial literacy–a review of literature. International Journal of Social Economics, 45(2), 437-444.
Nissenbaum, M., Raasch, B. J. and Ratner, C. L. (2004).
Ernst & Young's personal financial
planning guide. John Wiley & Sons. John Wiley & Sons
Noctor, M., Stoney, S. and Stradling, R. (1992).
Financial Literacy: A Discussion of Concepts and Competences of Financial
Literacy and Opportunities for its Introduction into Young People’s Learning. National Foundation for Education Research.
Opree, S.J., Buijzen, M., van Reijmersdal, E.A. and
Valkenburg ,P.M. (2011). Development and validation of the Material Values
Scale for children (MVS-c). Pers. and
Indiv Diff., 51(8), 963–968. <a
href="http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.029” target="_blank">
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.029 </a>
Organization for Economics Co-Operation and Development
(2005). Improving Financial Literacy:
Analysis of Issues and Policies. Paris, France.
Ratna, D., Al-shami, S. S. A., Rahim, B. R. A. and
Setya, M. F. (2018). Factors That Influence Financial Literacy On Small Medium
Enterprises: A Literature Review. Opción, 34(86), 1540-1557.
Refera, M. K., Dhaliwal, N. K. and Kaur, J. (2016). Financial literacy for developing countries in Africa: A
review of concept, significance and research opportunities. Journal of African Studies and development,
8(1), 1-12.
Remund, D. L. (2010). Financial Literacy Explicated:
The Case for a Clearer Definition in an Increasingly Complex Economy. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 44(2), 276-295.
Revuelta, J. and Kessel, D. (2007). Application of the
pi* goodness-of-fit index to latent structure models. Psicothema, 19(2), 322-328.
Richins, M. L. and Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values
orientation for materialism and its measurement: Scale development and
validation. Journal of consumer
research, 19(3), 303-316.
Roberts, J. A. and Sepulveda C. J. (1999). Demographics and money attitudes: a
test of Yamauchi and Templers (1982) money attitude scale in Mexico. Personality and individual Differences, 27(1),
19-35.
Selim, A. and Aydemir, S. D. (2014). A literature
review on financial literacy. Finansal Araştırmalar ve Çalışmalar Dergisi, 6(11). <a href=”http://doi.org/10.14784/JFRS.2014117326” target="_blank"> http://doi.org/10.14784/JFRS.2014117326 </a>
Spinella, M., Lester, D. and Yang, B. (2005). Consistency
of the Yamauchi/Templer money attitude scale. Psychological reports, 97(3), 962-962.
Spinella, M., Yang, B. and Lester, D. (2007).
Development of the executive personal finance scale. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117(3), 301-313.
Stango, V. and Zinman, J. (2009). Exponential growth
bias and household finance. The
Journal of Finance, 64(6), 2807-2849.
Steiger, J. H. and Lind, J. C. (1980).
Statistically-based tests for the number of common factors. Annual Spring Meeting of the Psychometric
Society. Iowa City, IA.
Taylor, S. and Wagland, S. (2011). Financial literacy:
A review of government policy and initiatives. Australasian Accounting, Business and Finance Journal, 5(2),
101-125.
Tommasi, M. and Busonera, A. (2012). Validation of
three compulsive buying scales on an italian sample. Psychological Reports, 111(3), 831-844. <a href=”http://doi.org/10.2466/03.15.20.PR0.111.6.831-844” target="_blank"> http://doi.org/10.2466/03.15.20.PR0.111.6.831-844 </a>
Torraco, R. J. (2005). Writing integrative literature
reviews: Guidelines and examples. Human
resource development review, 4(3), 356-367.
Torraco, R. J. (2016). Writing integrative literature
reviews: Using the past and present to explore the future. Human Resource Development Review, 15(4), 404-428.
Totenhagen, C. J., Casper, D. M., Faber, K. M., Bosch,
L. A., Wiggs, C. B. and Borden, L. M. (2015). Youth financial literacy: A
review of key considerations and promising delivery methods. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 36(2),
167-191
Vieira, E. (2012). What do we know about financial
literacy? A literature review. Marmara
Journal of European Studies, 20(2), 23–38.
Yamauchi, K. T. and Templer, D. J. (1982). The
development of a money attitude scale. Journal
of personality assessment, 46(5), 522-528.
Yang, B. and Lester, D. (2002). Internal consistency of
the Yamauchi/Templer money attitude scale. Psychological
reports, 91(3), 994-994.
Yang, B. and Lester, D. (2016). Validating the executive personal finance scale with financial investments
and expectations in university students. Psychological
Reports, 118(3), 804-809. <a href=”http://doi.org/10.1177/0033294115625581” target="_blank">http://doi.org/10.1177/0033294115625581</a>
Completamente de
acuerdo |
2 |
3 |
4 |
Completamente en
desacuerdo |
|
1.
Realizo cuidadosamente un presupuesto o plan de gastos |
|||||
2.
Asisto al supermercado con una lista de lo que voy a comprar |
|||||
3.
Evalúo e identifico mis hábitos de gasto con base en mis registros de consumo |
|||||
4. Llevo
un registro de mis ingresos, gastos, retiros de efectivo, etc. Adicional a lo
que proporciona mi banca en línea o estado de cuenta |
|||||
5.
Antes de recibir el estado de cuenta de mi tarjeta de crédito, sé exactamente
cuánt debo pagar para no generar intereses. |
|||||
6.
Cuando realizo compras a meses sin intereses, analizo que mi compra esté
dentro de mi presupuesto |
|||||
7.
Acostumbro a retirar efectivo de mi tarjeta de crédito |
|||||
8.
Acepto las tarjetas de crédito que me ofrecen bancos y tiendas |
|||||
9.
Acostumbro a pagar gastos de comida o despensa a meses sin intereses |
|||||
10.
Invierto en instrumentos financieros (p. ej. Acciones, fondos de inversión,
etc.) |
|||||
11.
Dedico tiempo a informarme sobre los mejores rendimientos para decidir en
cuáles instrumentos colocar mi dinero |
|||||
12.
Reviso y ajusto mis inversiones en un periodo no mayor a un año |
|||||
13.
Tengo disponible al menos 6 meses de mi sueldo en ahorros para utilizar ante
una emergencia (p. ej. Pérdida de empleo) |
|||||
14.
El ahorro es un renglón de mi presupuesto, siempre ahorro un porcentaje de mi
ingreso |
|||||
15.
Aparto dinero para mis metas (p. ej. Vacaciones, automóvil, educación) |
|||||
16.
Tengo un plan de aportaciones para mi pensión |
|||||
17.
Sé en dónde está mi AFORE y estoy consciente de los rendimientos que me
brinda |
|||||
18.
Realizo periódicamente aportaciones adicionales a mi plan de retiro |
Appendix 1. Personal
Finance Scale
Item |
Factor Number |
||||||||
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
|
Q23 |
0.679 |
0.008 |
-0.038 |
0.054 |
0.180 |
-0.259 |
0.099 |
0.098 |
0.195 |
Q22 |
0.675 |
0.100 |
-0.049 |
0.125 |
0.048 |
-0.094 |
0.061 |
0.238 |
0.295 |
RC Q9 |
-0.672 |
-0.101 |
-0.104 |
-0.076 |
0.017 |
-0.178 |
0.074 |
0.146 |
0.315 |
Q14 |
0.658 |
-0.117 |
0.098 |
0.220 |
0.023 |
0.092 |
0.239 |
-0.062 |
-0.180 |
Q25 |
0.636 |
0.130 |
0.156 |
0.060 |
0.079 |
-0.366 |
-0.019 |
0.051 |
0.022 |
Q16 |
0.064 |
0.758 |
0.078 |
0.092 |
0.207 |
-0.109 |
0.083 |
0.141 |
-0.064 |
Q18 |
0.099 |
0.665 |
0.121 |
0.059 |
0.332 |
-0.180 |
0.063 |
0.083 |
-0.142 |
Q17 |
0.197 |
0.569 |
0.323 |
-0.021 |
0.109 |
-0.116 |
0.132 |
-0.176 |
0.067 |
RC Q20 |
-0.348 |
0.525 |
0.010 |
0.356 |
-0.061 |
0.038 |
0.202 |
0.030 |
0.339 |
RC Q19 |
-0.083 |
0.507 |
-0.018 |
0.101 |
-0.106 |
0.324 |
0.003 |
0.361 |
0.275 |
RC Q21 |
-0.274 |
0.496 |
-0.143 |
0.309 |
-0.050 |
0.485 |
0.060 |
-0.044 |
-0.046 |
Q2 |
0.146 |
0.491 |
0.310 |
0.109 |
-0.114 |
0.222 |
0.009 |
0.046 |
-0.262 |
Q3 |
0.006 |
0.190 |
0.778 |
0.180 |
0.044 |
-0.019 |
-0.095 |
0.125 |
0.084 |
Q5 |
0.126 |
-0.044 |
0.776 |
0.104 |
0.025 |
0.045 |
0.085 |
-0.033 |
-0.106 |
Q1 |
-0.027 |
0.242 |
0.740 |
0.239 |
0.085 |
-0.216 |
0.152 |
-0.120 |
0.046 |
Q10 |
0.117 |
0.190 |
0.190 |
0.725 |
0.030 |
-0.139 |
0.059 |
0.037 |
-0.102 |
Q8 |
0.157 |
0.159 |
0.264 |
0.635 |
0.078 |
0.112 |
0.028 |
0.141 |
0.022 |
Q7 |
0.333 |
-0.023 |
0.179 |
0.475 |
0.301 |
0.069 |
-0.001 |
0.422 |
0.059 |
Q29 |
0.221 |
0.076 |
0.233 |
-0.011 |
0.769 |
0.025 |
0.098 |
0.072 |
-0.109 |
Q26 |
-0.112 |
0.319 |
-0.148 |
0.107 |
0.707 |
-0.010 |
0.093 |
-0.132 |
0.109 |
Q28 |
0.345 |
-0.020 |
0.060 |
0.405 |
0.519 |
-0.115 |
-0.195 |
0.155 |
-0.117 |
Q27 |
0.075 |
0.084 |
0.052 |
0.024 |
-0.004 |
-0.736 |
-0.019 |
-0.012 |
0.032 |
RC Q15 |
-0.055 |
-0.183 |
0.116 |
-0.077 |
0.047 |
0.413 |
-0.412 |
-0.271 |
0.192 |
Q13 |
0.112 |
0.124 |
0.117 |
0.013 |
0.120 |
-0.050 |
0.790 |
0.087 |
-0.134 |
Q12 |
0.336 |
-0.028 |
0.114 |
0.492 |
0.094 |
0.080 |
0.540 |
-0.055 |
-0.026 |
RC 24 |
-0.118 |
0.188 |
-0.012 |
-0.032 |
-0.082 |
0.363 |
0.457 |
-0.022 |
0.300 |
RC Q4 |
0.028 |
-0.044 |
0.123 |
-0.227 |
-0.005 |
0.159 |
-0.032 |
-0.728 |
0.055 |
RC Q6 |
0.229 |
0.165 |
0.331 |
-0.223 |
0.010 |
0.234 |
0.161 |
0.537 |
0.000 |
RC Q11 |
0.074 |
-0.088 |
0.018 |
-0.068 |
-0.045 |
0.019 |
-0.123 |
-0.047 |
0.836 |
Appendix 2. Factor Analysis,
29 items, 9 factors.
Item |
Factor Number |
|||||
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
|
Q1 |
0.790 |
0.251 |
0.097 |
0.015 |
0.033 |
0.035 |
Q3 |
0.783 |
0.026 |
0.214 |
0.087 |
-0.054 |
0.078 |
Q5 |
0.732 |
-0.089 |
0.085 |
-0.092 |
0.242 |
-0.004 |
Q2 |
0.393 |
0.184 |
0.103 |
0.261 |
0.278 |
-0.150 |
Q18 |
0.222 |
0.703 |
0.193 |
0.109 |
0.054 |
0.024 |
Q16 |
0.203 |
0.675 |
0.173 |
0.296 |
0.029 |
0.040 |
Q26 |
-0.094 |
0.550 |
0.296 |
0.037 |
-0.142 |
-0.120 |
Q17 |
0.450 |
0.468 |
-0.042 |
0.141 |
0.104 |
0.142 |
RC Q15 |
0.128 |
-0.456 |
0.046 |
-0.042 |
-0.236 |
-0.170 |
Q29 |
0.162 |
0.385 |
0.372 |
-0.223 |
0.193 |
0.035 |
Q27 |
0.099 |
0.380 |
-0.062 |
-0.337 |
-0.182 |
0.344 |
Q28 |
0.044 |
0.188 |
0.697 |
-0.285 |
0.105 |
0.135 |
Q7 |
0.120 |
0.012 |
0.690 |
0.086 |
0.200 |
0.259 |
Q8 |
0.310 |
0.027 |
0.588 |
0.229 |
0.168 |
0.019 |
Q10 |
0.276 |
0.182 |
0.557 |
0.090 |
0.159 |
-0.023 |
RC Q4 |
0.211 |
-0.132 |
-0.357 |
-0.198 |
0.022 |
-0.200 |
RC Q19 |
0.045 |
0.098 |
0.151 |
0.693 |
-0.117 |
0.071 |
RC Q20 |
0.146 |
0.308 |
0.110 |
0.635 |
-0.245 |
-0.073 |
RC Q21 |
-0.018 |
0.128 |
0.183 |
0.606 |
0.010 |
-0.454 |
RC 24 |
0.003 |
0.003 |
-0.193 |
0.579 |
0.141 |
-0.019 |
RC Q6 |
0.223 |
0.015 |
0.023 |
0.299 |
0.276 |
0.254 |
Q14 |
0.080 |
-0.073 |
0.234 |
-0.137 |
0.663 |
0.232 |
Q12 |
0.114 |
0.085 |
0.308 |
0.154 |
0.579 |
0.100 |
Q13 |
0.060 |
0.387 |
-0.135 |
0.201 |
0.563 |
0.062 |
RC Q9 |
-0.149 |
0.041 |
-0.195 |
0.185 |
-0.559 |
-0.103 |
Q22 |
-0.037 |
0.054 |
0.284 |
0.083 |
0.256 |
0.677 |
Q23 |
-0.038 |
0.157 |
0.218 |
-0.166 |
0.278 |
0.648 |
Q25 |
0.191 |
0.228 |
0.181 |
-0.256 |
0.251 |
0.549 |
RC Q11 |
0.047 |
-0.282 |
-0.051 |
0.235 |
-0.416 |
0.524 |
Appendix 3. Factor Analysis.
29 items. 6 Factors
Item |
Factor Number |
||||||
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
|
Q16 |
0.730 |
0.150 |
0.153 |
0.058 |
0.259 |
0.077 |
0.045 |
RC Q20 |
0.683 |
-0.301 |
-0.026 |
0.304 |
-0.123 |
0.041 |
0.035 |
Q18 |
0.608 |
0.164 |
0.176 |
0.070 |
0.382 |
0.037 |
0.192 |
RC Q19 |
0.594 |
0.082 |
0.032 |
0.091 |
-0.134 |
-0.153 |
-0.500 |
Q17 |
0.513 |
0.181 |
0.383 |
-0.088 |
0.134 |
0.156 |
0.158 |
RC Q21 |
0.511 |
-0.389 |
-0.096 |
0.222 |
0.000 |
0.111 |
-0.382 |
Q22 |
0.131 |
0.782 |
-0.056 |
0.186 |
-0.045 |
0.120 |
-0.068 |
Q23 |
-0.006 |
0.757 |
-0.032 |
0.102 |
0.124 |
0.138 |
0.091 |
Q25 |
0.031 |
0.689 |
0.215 |
0.078 |
0.105 |
0.072 |
0.218 |
Q3 |
0.154 |
0.086 |
0.810 |
0.175 |
0.028 |
-0.116 |
-0.095 |
Q5 |
-0.117 |
0.033 |
0.770 |
0.126 |
0.022 |
0.158 |
-0.007 |
Q1 |
0.215 |
-0.015 |
0.763 |
0.179 |
0.085 |
0.114 |
0.197 |
Q2 |
0.342 |
0.006 |
0.390 |
0.091 |
0.027 |
0.215 |
-0.197 |
Q10 |
0.190 |
0.030 |
0.197 |
0.731 |
0.038 |
0.111 |
0.205 |
Q8 |
0.195 |
0.096 |
0.236 |
0.665 |
0.016 |
0.152 |
-0.110 |
Q7 |
0.015 |
0.374 |
0.114 |
0.635 |
0.207 |
0.050 |
-0.122 |
Q29 |
-0.007 |
0.167 |
0.207 |
0.102 |
0.781 |
0.146 |
-0.005 |
Q26 |
0.328 |
-0.082 |
-0.115 |
0.035 |
0.707 |
0.041 |
-0.001 |
Q28 |
-0.095 |
0.345 |
0.065 |
0.504 |
0.517 |
-0.069 |
0.046 |
Q13 |
0.228 |
0.089 |
0.093 |
-0.083 |
0.090 |
0.771 |
0.020 |
Q12 |
0.031 |
0.131 |
0.082 |
0.411 |
0.056 |
0.707 |
-0.005 |
Q14 |
-0.208 |
0.430 |
0.116 |
0.207 |
0.050 |
0.538 |
-0.040 |
Q27 |
0.101 |
0.161 |
0.000 |
0.058 |
-0.015 |
-0.033 |
0.848 |
Appendix 4. Factor analysis.
23 items. 7 Factors
Item |
Factor Number |
|||||
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
|
Q1 |
0.799 |
0.090 |
0.008 |
0.140 |
0.159 |
0.116 |
Q3 |
0.798 |
0.124 |
0.075 |
0.201 |
0.016 |
-0.108 |
Q5 |
0.744 |
-0.153 |
-0.001 |
0.169 |
-0.047 |
0.158 |
Q17 |
0.453 |
0.318 |
0.268 |
-0.170 |
0.340 |
0.141 |
Q2 |
0.382 |
0.346 |
-0.010 |
0.099 |
0.060 |
0.239 |
RC Q19 |
0.029 |
0.724 |
0.096 |
0.065 |
-0.059 |
-0.127 |
RC Q20 |
0.076 |
0.714 |
-0.210 |
0.107 |
0.106 |
0.052 |
RC Q21 |
-0.087 |
0.650 |
-0.391 |
0.163 |
0.060 |
0.138 |
Q16 |
0.223 |
0.568 |
0.222 |
-0.011 |
0.477 |
0.084 |
Q22 |
-0.065 |
0.117 |
0.762 |
0.235 |
-0.045 |
0.148 |
Q23 |
-0.038 |
-0.110 |
0.742 |
0.169 |
0.117 |
0.148 |
Q25 |
0.231 |
-0.126 |
0.701 |
0.112 |
0.141 |
0.073 |
Q7 |
0.094 |
0.062 |
0.313 |
0.696 |
0.109 |
0.076 |
Q10 |
0.260 |
0.204 |
0.041 |
0.635 |
0.087 |
0.131 |
Q28 |
0.040 |
-0.173 |
0.281 |
0.618 |
0.406 |
-0.057 |
Q8 |
0.258 |
0.279 |
0.084 |
0.615 |
0.009 |
0.170 |
Q26 |
-0.112 |
0.151 |
-0.091 |
0.105 |
0.748 |
0.040 |
Q29 |
0.167 |
-0.210 |
0.113 |
0.256 |
0.700 |
0.135 |
Q18 |
0.245 |
0.388 |
0.227 |
0.025 |
0.578 |
0.039 |
Q13 |
0.119 |
0.129 |
0.111 |
-0.125 |
0.184 |
0.764 |
Q12 |
0.094 |
0.046 |
0.105 |
0.382 |
0.042 |
0.718 |
Q14 |
0.074 |
-0.209 |
0.358 |
0.290 |
-0.059 |
0.557 |
Appendix 5. Factor Analysis.
22 items. 6 dimensions.
Observed |
|
Unobserved |
Estimate |
Variable |
Construct |
|
|
Q1 |
<--- |
E |
0.819 |
Q2 |
<--- |
E |
0.403 |
Q3 |
<--- |
E |
0.766 |
Q4 |
<--- |
E |
0.607 |
Q5 |
<--- |
E |
0.424 |
Q6 |
<--- |
CC |
0.519 |
Q7 |
<--- |
CC |
0.53 |
Q8 |
<--- |
CC |
0.714 |
Q9 |
<--- |
CC |
0.604 |
Q10 |
<--- |
INV |
0.656 |
Q11 |
<--- |
INV |
0.76 |
Q12 |
<--- |
INV |
0.655 |
Q13 |
<--- |
S |
0.623 |
Q14 |
<--- |
S |
0.718 |
Q15 |
<--- |
S |
0.63 |
Q16 |
<--- |
R |
0.752 |
Q17 |
<--- |
R |
0.467 |
Q18 |
<--- |
R |
0.588 |
Q19 |
<--- |
INS |
0.713 |
Q20 |
<--- |
INS |
0.484 |
Appendix 6. Structural
equation model results 1
Unobserved |
AVE |
Construct |
|
E |
0.3936302 |
CC |
0.35621825 |
INV |
0.478987 |
S |
0.43351767 |
R |
0.37644567 |
INS |
0.3713125 |
Appendix 7. AVE 1
Unobserved |
Composite |
Construct |
Reliability |
E |
0.75038724 |
CC |
0.68510822 |
INV |
0.73290881 |
S |
0.69567347 |
R |
0.6357681 |
INS |
0.53260632 |
Appendix 8. Composite
Reliability 1
[1] Lic. en
Contaduría Pública y Finanzas; Estudiante del Doctorado en Ciencias
Administrativas; EGADE Business School; Instituto
Tecnológico y de Estudios Superiores de Monterrey; Líneas de investigación: Economía
y Finanzas; nuria.rojas18@gmail.com.
[2] Máster en Finanzas;
Estudiante del Doctorado en Ciencias Administrativas; EGADE Business School; Instituto Tecnológico y de Estudios Superiores de
Monterrey; Líneas de investigación: Economía y Finanzas; juliovga@gmail.com